Language Development: Pragmatic development

Aridem Vintoni

Pragmatics can be defined as the intentional use of language to interact with other people. Although this definition emphasizes language, the use of language for social ends involves much more, including the coordination of linguistic information with gestures, facial expression, eye gaze, and body posture; the use of information from the physical, social, and affective context of the talk to decide what to say, how to say it, and what another person's words mean; and the integration of the current talk with relevant information from past encounters with the other participants or from previous events and entities referred to in the current talk. Moreover, typical children engage in considerable intentional communication prior to language, relying on nonverbal behavior and situational support. Consequently, becoming pragmatically competent requires skills and knowledge beyond those entailed in the acquisition of the linguistic system, including memory skills, deep and well-organized knowledge about the social and physical worlds and about the communicative process itself, the ability to flexibly integrate multiple sources of information from different modalities, and the ability to plan and recognize goal-directed sequences of actions.

 

EARLY SPEECH ACTS IN CHILD LANGUAGE

 

The early development of the communicative functions speech acts of child’s language occurs simultaneously with the development of the linguistic aspects of child language. Yasin (1991: 218) states that the early communicative functions of child language or speech acts i.e. illocutionary acts are determined by three kinds of intonation: “terminal”, “interrogative”, and “continuous” intonation. In addition, Clark and Clark (1977: 312) children’s speech acts are determined by the gesture accompanying their utterances. Two groups of gestures are “pointing”, and “reaching”. These two groups of gestures are considered as the precursors to speech acts of “asserting” and “requesting”.

1.  Assertions

To communicate with their parents and other adults, at the single-word utterances, children begin to produce single words accompanying with certain gestures such as “bye-bye + (hand waves)” or “boo + (peeping from behind hand)”. These early gestures and word combinations tend to be part of a routine that adults insist on in a certain context. The first assertions in the child language development usually consist of a general “deictic” or “pointing” words (like “there” or “that”) combined with a gesture such as: “Da + (point)” (in single-word utterances) and “see boy + (point)” (in two-word utterances).

2.  Requests

Request is one of speech acts that most frequently occur in various social interactions. A request is made when an addressor asks the addressee to “do” something (Vintoni: 2009). It is a directive speech act which counts as an attempt to get the addressee to do an act that the addressor wants the addressee to do, in which the addressor believes that the addressee is able to do, and which it is not obvious that the addressee will do in the normal course of events or of the addressee’s own accord. 
 
fianou (1992) in Fukushima (1996) states that request can be categorized into two: requests for action and request for information. He notes that request for action involves a higher degree of imposition than requests for information. He states that requests for an action refer to naming the object that the child wants to be given, and requests for information tend to be in form of questions. In making request (when they want the candy) children might say “give candy + (pointing)”. In addition, children might ask questions demanding a simple “yes” or “no” answer by using “rising” intonation such “Sit water?”. Another requests for information might be in forms of “Where” questions such as “Where doggie go?”, “Where mama?”, “Where ball?”. Besides, children also tend to make some negative requests such as “No more” (don’t do that anymore). 
 
Accordingly, in relation to the development of child language, Clark and Clark (1977: 314-315) list some examples of both speech acts (“asserting” and “requesting”) produced by the children in both in the single-word and two-word utterances stages of child language development. They are as follow:

Assertion:

Ba + (look)
(Looking at the ball)
 
Dada + (look)
(Looking at father)
 
See boy. See sock. That car.
(Presence of object)
 
Allgone shoe. No wet. Byebye hot.
(Denial of presence)
 
Bill here. There doggie.
(Location of object)
 
My milk. Kendall chair. Mama dress.
(Possession of object)
 
Pretty boat. Big bus.
(Quality of object)
 
Mommy sleep. Hit ball. Block fall.
(Ongoing event)

 

Request:
 
Mama + (whine) + (reach)
(Reaches towards any object desired)
 
Ma + (whine) + (point)
( Pointing at microphone)
 
More taxi. Want gum. Where ball?
(Request for action)
 
Where doggies go? Sit water? 
(Request for information)
 
Early from the age of around six months, children were able to communicate with their parents and other adults; they are able to “assert” or “request” their interlocutors, although in forms of incomplete syntactic structure of sentences. They perform speech acts with the combination of their utterances and certain gestures and intonations. The communicative functions of language i.e. speech acts produced by children develop simultaneously together with the development child linguistic competence. They developed together with the development of linguistic aspects (phonemes or sounds, morpheme, words, and sentences) of language, through a process involving some stages.